Sunday, July 8, 2007

The Myth of Greek language 'Purity'


Classification of the European Language Families by Genetic Distance

Rosalind M. Harding, and Robert R. Sokal

PNAS 1988;85;9370-9372
doi:10.1073/pnas.85.23.9370

This information is current as of October 2006.

E-mail Alerts

Rights & Permissions

Reprints

This article has been cited by other articles:
www.pnas.org/otherarticles

Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top
right corner of the article or click here.

To reproduce this article in part (figures, tables) or in entirety, see:
www.pnas.org/misc/rightperm.shtml

To order reprints, see:
www.pnas.org/misc/reprints.shtml

Notes:

Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA
Vol. 85, pp. 9370-9372, December 1988
Population Biology

Classification of the European language families by genetic distance
(human variation/gene frequencies)

ROSALIND M. HARDING AND ROBERT R. SOKAL
Department of Ecology and Evolution, State University of New York at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794-5245

Contributed by Robert R. Sokal, August 24, 1988

ABSTRACT
Genetic distances among speakers of the European language families were computed by using gene- frequency data for human blood group antigens, enzymes, and proteins of26 genetic systems.
Each system was represented by a different subset of 3369 localities across Europe. By subject- ing the matrix ofdistances to numerical taxonomic procedures, we obtained a grouping of the language families of Europe by their genetic distances as contrasted with their linguistic relationships.
The resulting classification largely reflects geographic propinquity rather than linguistic origins. This is evidence for the primary importance ofshort-range interdemic gene flow in shaping the modern gene pools of Europe.
Yet, some language families-i.e., Basque, Finnic (including Lappish), and Semitic (Maltese)-have distant genetic relation- ships with their geographic neighbors. These results indicate that European gene pools still reflect the remote origins ofsome ethnic units subsumed by these major linguistic groups.


The aim of analyses of current gene frequency patterns is to infer the microevolutionary processes that have generated these patterns.
Such inferences are facilitated when the investigator can employ other relevant variables in the analysis. In human populations such variables are geographic distance and language.
Genetic similarity may be due to (i) geographic proximity or (ii) relationships reflected by lan- guage phylogeny. Ifgenetic relations among languages reflect their linguistic origins, we expect strong congruence between genetic affinities and linguistic relationships.
(The common origin and phylogenetic divergence ofseveral ofthe language families of Europe is well established, see ref. 1.) Alternatively, if genetic affinities between language families are inversely proportional to spatial distance, they may be attributed to localized gene flow. This is Malecot's isolation- by-distance model (2), which assumes stochastic divergence of populations from a common origin. The fit of these alternative models will be tested by comparing the observed genetic distances between pairs of language-family regions with (i) their spatial distances and (ii) their linguistic distances.


We prefer, with Lalouel (3), to calculate genetic distances based on a minimum ofgenetic assumptions. Generally, most calculations of genetic distances among human populations are highly correlated (4), giving good reason to choose the simplest computational method. However, one particular feature of the data set on which this study is based requires special attention. To examine variability on a continental scale, it was necessary to combine data from a large number of independent studies and, as a result, each genetic system
is based on a different sampling scheme. Although there is
some overlap in the sampling localities for different genetic systems, the final data matrix is unbalanced by the absence of observations at a given locality for various genetic sys- tems. Thus, genetic distances had to be computed separately

The publication costs ofthis article were defrayed in part by page charge
payment. This article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement"
in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact.
for each genetic system among the particular set of locality
samples representing that system.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The 12 living language families in Europe fall into five language phyla as follows (1):
Indo-European (Albanian, Baltic, Celtic, Germanic, Greek, Romance, and Slavic);
Finno-Ugric [Finnic and Ugric (Hungarian)];
Altaic (Turkic);
Afro-Asiatic [Semitic (Maltese)]; and
Language Isolates (Basque).
A linguistic distance matrix of language-family relationships was constructed by setting the Baltic-Slavic distance to 1 (these are the only two Indo-European families for which close genetic affinities are generally accepted, see ref. 1), all other distances between language families within
a phylum to 2, and distances between language families
belonging to different phyla to 4. Thus, language distances mostly contrast intraphylum and interphylum distances.
A geographic distance matrix between all pairs oflanguage
families was computed from great-circle distances between subjectively chosen centers of language-family regions. Genetic distances were calculated by using frequency data for human blood antigens, enzymes, and proteins of 26 genetic systems, each for a different subset of 3369 localities across Europe.
Because of the different spatial sampling for each genetic system, we computed genetic distances sepa- rately for each system. All localities were also assigned a language-family affiliation.
The systems and the sources of the data are described elsewhere (5-7).
Sample sizes range from 50 to many thousands ofpersons. Previous work (5) has shown that the simplest of these distances, that due to Prevosti et al. (8), provided essentially the same information
as more elaborate formulations.
It was, therefore, adopted here.
To allowforpossible bias due to different ranges ofgene frequencies, we also standardized the distances.
For each system we first calculated genetic distances for all pairs oflocalities and subsequently averaged over all locality pairs representing a particular pairwise combination of language families.
This yielded, for a given system, an average genetic distance for each pair oflanguage families. However, for some systems, we lacked localities to represent one or more language families and could not compute distances for certain pairwise combinations of language families.
This resulted in genetic distance matrices for some systems with missing values for some pairwise comparisons. Since different genetic systems are based on different sets of localities, the particular pairwise combinations missing in the genetic distance matrices vary among systems.
The final genetic distance matrix (Table 1) was obtained by averaging over all systems.
Nine of the language families are well represented by genetic systems, but genetic distances for Semitic, Baltic, and Albanian are based on only seven, three, and two systems, respectively.
These few systems may furnish unreliable estimates of distances between language families. For this reason, we analyze both the reduced set of 9 language families and the total set of 12. Our conclusions are based

largely on the 9 families, with added consideration of the larger data set when appropriate.
The average genetic distance matrix between pairs of language families was subjected to standard numerical tax- onomic clustering and ordination procedures (9).
Hierarchic classifications of the language families were achieved by UPGMA (unweighted pair-group) clustering of the average distance matrices (9).
Ordinations ofthese distance matrices were obtained by nonmetric multidimensional scaling in three dimensions.
All computations were carried out by the NTSYS program (10).
Congruence between genetic, linguistic, and geographic distances was tested for significance as described (5).
Using methods ofquadratic assignment (11), we calculated pairwise Mantel matrix correlations (12, 13) and investigated three- way relations between the distance matrices by computing partial correlations (14).
These correlations were tested for significance by Monte Carlo permutation methods. These computations were carried out by using the R package for multivariate data analysis (15).

RESULTS

Fig. 1 shows the hierarchic clustering ofgenetic distances for 9 language families.
A Germanic-Celtic cluster isjoined later by Romance, and a Slavic-Ugric cluster isjoined by Turkic.
Finnic, Basque, and Greek are outliers to these clusters.
Including Albanian, Baltic, and Semitic in the analysis changes the phenogram by affiliating Greek with Albanian, Baltic with Turkic, and clustering Germanic-Celtic with Slavic-Ugric before adding Romance. Semitic, Basque, and Finnic are outliers to the clusters of 12 language families.
An ordination ofthe genetic distance matrix (Fig. 2) depicts the relative genetic distances between the nine language families.
Finnic and Basque are outliers at opposite ends of the ordinated space.
The Celtic-Germanic and Slavic-Ugric language-family pairs are evident along the first axis which runs roughly East-West.
The second axis approximates a North-South gradient.
In the minimum spanning tree, Turkic links Ugric and Greek with Romance.
The position of Romance is central on the first and second axes, but isolated by the third, explaining its variable affiliation during cluster-
ing.
Genetic distance (Gen) correlates significantly with geog- raphy (Geo) but not with language (Lan). The pairwise correlations of distance matrices based on nine language families are as follows: Gen x Geo = 0.468 (P <>
x Lan = 0.182 (P>0.05), and Geo x Lan = 0.177 (P>0.05).
The partial correlations are (Gen x Geo)-Lan = 0.451 (P <
0.01) and (Gen x Lan)-Geo = 0.114 (P > 0.05). Geography
determines 20.3% of the variance of the genetic distances, language determines only 1.0%o, and factors common to geography and language determine 1.6%.
One might have expected a high and significant Gen x Lan correlation, because speakers ofa particular language (family) tend to be found settled near each other.
In other words, when geographic distances between samples are small we expect linguistic distances to be small, and vice versa.
Ifgeography is likewise correlated with genetics, then genetic and linguistic distances should also be positively correlated.
But the Geo x Lan correlation in this study is low because centers of
language phyla are positioned in Europe both relatively close and far apart spatially causing the relationship between geography and language to break down.
Therefore, the common effect ofgeography does not produce a high correlation between genetics and language.
This finding contrasts with the significant correlation between genetic and language distances reported by Sokal (5).
In that study correlations among genetics, linguistics, and geography were calculated for pairwise locality distances. Since a finer scale ofdistances was used, both the correlation ofgeography and of language and the common effect of geography were greater.

DISCUSSION

This study shows that genetic distances between the European language families do not reflect their accepted linguistic relationships.
If we group the language families by their linguistic origins, there should be a cluster of the Indo- European language families, Baltic and Slavic being most closely related, a separate branch for the Finnic and Ugric speakers, and separate coordinate branches for the Turkic, Semitic, and Basque language families.
The genetic distances of some interphylum language-family pairs, such as those between Slavic and Ugric speakers, or between Turkic and Ugric speakers, however, are closer than some distances within a phylum, as between Greek and Celtic speakers or between Finnic and Ugric speakers.
The low matrix correlation between genetics and language confirms the lack of agreement between presumed language phylogeny and the observed genetic distances.
If genetic distances reflect geographic proximity, we should be able to predict genetic affinity from a clustering of the great-circle distances between language families.
This yields Germanic-Celtic and Romance-Basque as mutually closest pairs, with the two pairs together forming a major cluster.
Likewise, Slavic-Ugric and Greek-Turkic both cluster as pairs.
Finally, Finnic is an outlier.
Clustering all 12 language families by great-circle distance enlarges the Greek Turkic cluster into one that also includes Albanian and Semitic and places Baltic-Finnic as an outlying pair. Considerable concordance between geographic proximity and observed genetic relations is evident and confirmed by the significant correlation between geographic and genetic dis- tances.
The role of geography can also be seen in the ordinations.
Disregarding the outliers, Fig. 2 demonstrates an East-West separation of geographically adjacent language family pairs, Germanic-Celtic from Turkic-Greek with Slavic-Ugric intermediate.
The small Greek-Albanian genetic distance is also consistent with geographic proximity, although we attach less confidence to this value.
The observations made here are supported by a largely geographic clustering of European map quadrats characterized by gene frequencies (16).
A geographic gene flow model does not, however, explain why the Basque and Finnic language families are outliers both in the ordinations and phenograms, nor why Semitic is an outlier in the extended dataset.
These results reflect the distant origins of speakers of these language groups.
The Finnic language family is given its unique genetic profile by inclusion of the Lapps.
These populations, ethnically different from other Finnic speakers, apparently migrated to northern Scandinavia from northern Eurasia (17).
The Basques have long been an isolated enclave, presumably descended from the pre-Indo-European inhabitants of Europe (18, 19).
The Semitic speakers have North African origins.
These results suggest that some modification of the strictly geographic gene flow model by language origin may provide greater concordance with the genetic relationships between language families.
We conclude that affinities between modern European
gene pools have been formed primarily by relatively short- range gene flow between geographically adjacent populations.
Yet, between the speakers of some language families and their geographic neighbors, there are genetic differences that apparently reflect their remote historical and linguistic origins.

We thank Barbara Thomson for computational assistance. Prof. L. L. Cavalli-Sforza and Dr. Neal Oden provided helpful comments.
This research was supported by Grant GM28262 from the National Institutes of Health.
This article is contribution 681 in Ecology and Evolution from the State University of New York at Stony Brook.


1. Ruhlen, M. (1987) A Guide to the World's Languages (Stanford Univ. Press, Stanford, CA), Vol. 1.
2. Maldcot, G. (1948) Les Mathematiques de l'Hewredite6 (Masson et Cie, Paris) [Maldcot, G., trans. (1969) The Mathematics of
Heredity (Freeman, San Francisco)].
3. Lalouel, J.-M. (1980) in Current Developments in Anthropological Genetics, eds. Mielke, J. H. & Crawford, M. H. (Plenum, New York), Vol. 1, pp. 209-250.
4. Jorde, L. B. (1985) Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 14, 343-373.
5. Sokal, R. R. (1988) Proc. NatI. Acad. Sci. USA 85, 1722-1726.
6. Sokal, R. R., Oden, N. L. & Thomson, B. A. (1988) Am. J.
7. Sokal, R. R., Oden, N. L., Legendre, P., Fortin, M. J., Kim,
8. Prevosti, A., Ocana, J. & Alonso, G. (1975) Theor. Appl. Genet. 45, 231-241.
9. Sneath, P. H. A. & Sokal, R. R. (1973) Numerical Taxonomy (Freeman, San Francisco).
10. Rohlf, F. J. (1985) Numerical Taxonomy System ofMultivariate Statistical Programs, Technical Report (State Univ. of New York, Stony Brook).Hubert, L. (1987) Assignment
Methods in Combinatorial Data
12.
13.

Analysis (Dekker, New York).
Mantel, N. (1967) Cancer Res. 27, 209-220.
Sokal, R. R. (1979) Syst. Zool. 28, 227-231.
14. Smouse, P. E., Long, J. C. & Sokal, R. R. (1986) Syst. Zool. 35, 627-632.
15. Legendre, P. (1985) The R Package for Multivariate Data Analysis, Technical Report (Universitd de Montreal, Mon- treal).
16. Derish, P. A. & Sokal, R. R. (1988) Hum. Biol., 60, 801-824.
17. Bunak, V. V. (1976) in Rassengeschichte der Menschheit, ed.
18.
19.

Schwidetzky, I. (Oldenbourg, Munich), Vol. 4, pp. 7-101.
Allieres, J. (1986) Les Basques (Univ. of France Press, Paris).
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1987) The Basque Population and An-
cient Migrations in Europe, Second World Basque Congress, in
press

No comments: